Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliff: NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 53(1), 153-164.
Chiavarino, C., Bugiani, S., Grandi, E., & Colle, L. (2013). Is automatic imitation based on goal coding or movement coding? A comparison of goal-directed and goal-less actions. Experimental Psychology, 60(3), 213.
Cole, G. G., Atkinson, M. A., D'Souza, A. D., Welsh, T. N., & Skarratt, P. A. (2017). Are goal states represented during kinematic imitation?
Cracco, E., Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Genschow, O., Rigoni, D., De Coster, L., & Brass, M. (2018). Automatic imitation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 144(5), 453.
Fazeli, D., & Moradi, N. (2017). The effect of task constraint on learning of movement pattern and parameter during observational learning. Motor Behavior, 8 (26), 17-34.
Forbes, P. A., & Hamilton, A. F. d. C. (2017). Moving higher and higher: imitators’ movements are sensitive to observed trajectories regardless of action rationality. Experimental Brain Research, 235(9), 2741-2753.
Gray, J. T., Neisser, U., Shapiro, B. A., & Kouns, S. (1991). Observational learning of ballet sequences: The role of kinematic information. Ecological Psychology, 3(2), 121-134.
Hayes, S. J., Ashford, D., & Bennett, S. J. (2008). Goal-directed imitation: The means to an end. Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 407-415.
Hayes, S. J., Hodges, N. J., Huys, R., & Williams, A. M. (2007). End-point focus manipulations to determine what information is used during observational learning. Acta Psychologica, 126(2), 120-137.
Hodges, N. J., Hayes, S. J., Breslin, G., & Williams, A. M. (2005). An evaluation of the minimal constraining information during observation for movement reproduction. Acta Psychologica, 119(3), 264-282.
Horn, R. R., Williams, A. M., & Scott, M. A. (2002). Learning from demonstrations: The role of visual search during observational learning from video and point-light models. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20(3), 253-269.
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14(2), 201-211.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 838.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant behavior and development, 17(1), 83-99.
Rumiati, R., & Tessari, A. (2002). Imitation of novel and well-known actions. Experimental Brain Research, 142(3), 425-433.
Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. D. (2005). Motor control and learning: a behavioral approach. Human Kinetics, Champaign.
Scully, D., & Newell, K. (1985). Observational-learning and the acquisition of motor-skills-toward a visual-perception perspective. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 11(4), 169-186.
Sidaway, B., Heise, G., & SchoenfelderZohdi, B. (1995). Quantifying the variability of angle-angle plots. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 29(4), 181-197.
Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2010). The influence of goals on movement kinematics during imitation. Experimental Brain Research, 204(3), 353-360.
Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(8), 1739-1749.
Williams, J. G., Davids, K., & Williams, A. M. (1999). Visual Perception and Action in Sport. Routledge.
Winter, D. A. (1990). Biomechanics and motor control of human movement: John Wiley & Sons.
Wohlschläger, A., Gattis, M., & Bekkering, H. (2003). Action generation and action perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 501-515.